Was WWI dazzle battleship camouflage actually effective?

During the First World War, British and US ships were painted in high contrast “dazzle” patterns, which were believed to confuse submariners aiming torpedoes at them by distorting the perception of the vessel’s heading .

But new research suggests the strategy may have only provided protection for fast moving, distant ships. It appears this week in the journal Royal Society Open Science.

A black and white photograph of a large steam powered ship painted in an alternating checkered pattern
RMS Mauretania painted in dazzle camouflage bringing troops home from Europe, 8 December 1918. Credit: New-York Tribune, public domain

“We investigated the effects of different camouflage patterns, including versions used in the war, on the perceived direction of travel for a 3-dimensional computer model of the RMS Mauretania,” the authors of the paper write.

The RMS Mauretania was used as a troop ship during WWI.

The 16 participants in the study viewed the simulated ship through a periscope-like window replicating real conditions, such as distance and periscope height. The digitised ships appeared pointed in a randomised direction and were coloured either a neutral grey or 1 of 5 different camouflage patterns.

“We found both benefits and problems associated with the dazzle approach,” the authors write, “particularly due to the interactive effects of hysteresis and twist.”

An image of 6 computer generated ship images facing parallel to the viewer. The ships are coloured grey, or black and white patterns of irregular circles, regular circles decreasing in size, splotches, and 2 different versions of the mauretania dazzle with checkers and stripes
The six camouflage conditions used in the psychophysical experiment (five patterns and neutral grey). These static ships point to the right/east, implying a direction of travel of 90°. Credit: Lovell et al 2024, R. Soc. Open Sci. DOI: 10.1098/rsos.240624

They found dazzle camouflage patterns that incorporated texture gradients “twisted” the perceived direction of the ship. However, another “hysteresis” effect made it look like ships were moving across the horizon rather than towards or away from the viewer, regardless of dazzle camouflage.

Interestingly, this hysteresis effect decreased as participants’ years of sailing experience increased. Though the reason for this remains unclear.

Newsletter

“We cannot know whether subtle visual cues were more readily available to experienced participants compared to others, or whether they simply put more effort into giving precise responses,” the authors write.

Rsos. 240624. F003 850
Screen capture of the experiment in progress. Participants viewed the stimulus ship through a binocular-like window (top left). An overhead “chart” (right) featured a submarine icon at the bottom always facing upwards (north/0°) with the response ship placed above that. Participants rotated the response ship on the right until it matched the perceived direction of travel for the stimulus ship on the left. In this example, the stimulus ship carries the simple-dazzle colour scheme and has a compass heading of 40° (NE). Credit: Lovell et al 2024, R. Soc. Open Sci. DOI: 10.1098/rsos.240624

In separate torpedo aiming computer simulations the researchers showed that, depending on the heading of the ship, the effects of twist and hysteresis combined to either enhance or diminish their protection from accurate torpedo aiming.

“Our work suggests that many WWI dazzle patterns would have been of limited benefit in systematically deceiving the enemy about a target ship’s direction,” the authors conclude.

“Indeed, our application of [the] pattern for RMS Mauretania produced only 1° more twist than the neutral grey condition.”

Instead, the camouflage that provided the most twist was that of regular circles increasing in size in a gradient across the length of the ship.

“If fast ships were painted with camouflage that introduced a strong twist, it might have introduced targeting errors for experienced submariners which could have saved lives.”

Sign up to our weekly newsletter



Related Content

[Botany • 2024] Aeschynanthus chayangtajoensis (Gesneriaceae) • A New Species from Arunachal Pradesh, India

How Bad Bacteria Hijack Your Gut and Trigger Painful Contractions

Unusual Activity in Our Guts Could Have Helped Our Brains Grow Larger : ScienceAlert

Leave a Comment